By Manuel Espinal
Manuel Espinal |
With
respect to learners’ age, it is well known that adults and younger learners can
learn through the use of explicit and implicit rules of the language. Thus, Explicit
rules refer to those grammatical rules that are learned as a result of an
instructional process, and learners, most of the times, are familiar with the
terminology applied to nominate such rules, for example: subject, predicate,
noun phrase, head of a noun phrase, verbal phrase, verb phrase, etc., which
both adults and younger learners can use them as reference for giving any
explanation with regard to that point. On
the other hand, implicit rules are referred to as those that learners or
speakers use and respect while speaking or writing, but they are unfamiliar
with the terminology for referring to those rules.
Along
this line, we need to determine the learners’ language background. However,
with regard to the learners’ language background, we must ponder two very
important factors: the setting (context) and the abilities of learners. We
refer to those learners who live where English, in this case, is spoken
(English as a second language) or those where the classroom is the most
suitable environment for performing that language (English as a foreign language).
Considering
that our primary focus is the role of grammar in writing in a second language
(English) researches (see Reid, 1998b and Ferris and Hedgcock, 2004) on that
issue claim that those learners who have been exposed to the formality of
classroom (EFL or international students) tend to commit less errors in their
written texts in comparison to those who has been learning English exposed to
the informality of everyday speech (ESL), because the first ones have been
educated under the grammar teaching format.
Further
to what has been said on the issue, it is imperative to say that students
learning English with no or less contact with native speakers use continuously
grammatical rules and they keep the formality for writing even for orality,
when it is well known that the repertoire for speaking a language is very
reduced in comparison to the register for writing. Also, teachers for long time
remain detecting and correcting errors when the setting is a country where
English is not spoken for everyday activities and the teaching process is
completely centered on the classroom setting. In a situation like that,
students are regularly exposed to an explicit grammar instruction.
Observing
the role of teachers in respect of the errors correctness via explicit rules,
there are opposing points of view in which some researchers cast doubts on the
effectiveness of correction for improving writers’ accuracy through the time (see
Truscott, 1996) and those researchers who argue that is very difficult for
adults to be effective if they do not receive attention for their writing
activities (see Ferris, 1999, 2011).
Stephen Krashen |
According to Ferris through explicit instruction, adult learners may
reduce errors and improve their writing at short-term. By the same line of preserving the influence
of correctness in students’ drafts, Silva (1993) maintains that the
effectiveness of correction is a process, and therefore it takes its time. And,
naturally, Stephen Krashen (1982) with his Acquisition-learning hypothesis and
Monitor hypothesis, who is one of the most critical researcher to the
intervention of explicit grammar instruction in the natural process of
acquisition of the second language. Krashen see no role of the form-focused instruction
for developing skills writers of a second language, on the contrary, he argues
that kind of instruction impede the learners’ communicative competences, and
nowadays grammar instruction must be focused on developing learners’ abilities
for communication, for fluently conveying accurate information.
And
finally, the tasks assigned by teachers must consider the proficiency level of
learners. In the early stage of
instruction, the terms to be used must be simple and easy to deal with or
understand which turn to be more complex as learners advance in learning grammar
rules. The tasks must not be at random, but planned with relation to the
learners’ knowledge background and ability to discriminate most of the rules
through their formation process.
Activities such as texts analysis through
reading and composing papers must be frequently (that is what I call: practice)
developed from basic structures to complex structures. Those tasks or
activities might be developed individually or in groups. The tasks might
consist of verb tenses combinations, use of the appropriate connectors
(cohesive devices) for making transitions from one paragraph to another and
giving cohesion and coherence to the written texts, use of the correct clause
for giving support to the previous proposition, use of lexical ties such as
synonyms and antonyms, etc.
No comments:
Post a Comment